CR2
Find us on
  • Home
  • About Us
  • Calendar
  • Classes
    • Fall 2012 Classes >
      • Reef Biology
      • Caribbean History
    • Spring 2013 Classes >
      • The Age of U. S. Imperialism
      • CR2: Coral Reefs on Camera
    • Marine Biology (Spring 2014)
    • Related Classes
  • Shedd Aquarium
    • Labs at the Shedd Aquarium
    • Underwater Robotics Club
  • With Every Drop

Cuttlefish courtship: smaller suitors seal the deal through subterfuge

8/2/2014

0 Comments

 
Picture
The mourning cuttlefish (Sepia plangon), a species of Australian cuttlefish
Problem-solving, forethought, and advanced decision-making are generally regarded as the domain of large vertebrate animals like primates and whales.  But cephalopods, a unique class of mollusks that have roamed the seas for five hundred million years, give higher mammals a run for their money when it comes to displays of high-level cognition. 
PictureCephalopod diversity
Cephalopods – squids, octopuses, cuttlefish, and nautiluses – live in a murky underwater world where prey is scarce and predators are plentiful (and, with the exception of the nautilus, these soft-bodied critters have no shell or other means of physical protection from a hungry shark or seal). So they've developed a variety of fantastic and bizarre adaptations that make them both deadly predators and elusive prey, chief among them the ability to change their color and texture; keen eyesight; powerful arms and beaks; and, as we've only begun to discover, a degree of intelligence that some behavioral researchers say is comparable to that of a dog or cat. Cephalopods have impressed us time and time again with their problem-solving capabilities; just a few examples include veined octopuses' calculated use of coconut shells for self-protection, communication and coordination between hunting Humboldt squid, and captive octopuses' many demonstrations of play-like behavior. Now researchers at Macquarie University have discovered that male mourning cuttlefish use their ability to change color, traditionally a means of misleading predators and prey, to trick members of their own species in their quest to find a mate. 

Mourning cuttlefish (Sepia plangon), generally solitary animals, gather together by the thousands once a year to mate. These groups' sex ratios are decidedly male-biased, and so males battle fiercely for the change to snag a mate – and once they have one, they guard her closely, preventing any competitors from stealing her away. But smaller, younger cuttlefish that are less capable of winning a mate in the traditional cuttlefish fashion have an ingenious way of making sure their genes get passed on. Much in the same manner as Homer's Odysseus slipped the Greeks into Troy in a wooden horse, smaller and weaker male mourning cuttlefish are able to approach and in many cases successfully inseminate an already-spoken-for female by posing as a female themselves. The male will split his skin coloration down the middle; the flank facing towards the female he's wooing bears the smooth, striped pattern characteristic of a male, while the flank facing away from his would-be mate – towards the male he's cheating – will flash yellow and mottled, the mark of a female cuttlefish. 

Picture
Macquarie researchers Culum Brown, Martin Garwood, and Jane Williamson explored the frequency of this behavior through observations of cuttlefish in the wild, taking photographs of mating mourning cuttlefish during SCUBA dives in Australia's Sydney Harbor. Over the course of six years, they assembled a photo collection of 108 groups of mating cuttlefish. They observed that males employed this trick fairly often, but only within a certain context. Males would only "cross-dress" when in the presence of a single female and a single competitor male. The same was seen in cuttlefish kept in a captive enclosure. In both cases, the tactic was generally successful; cuckolding males managed to slip their sperm past.

Sending false messages to other individuals – or lying – is something we may be accustomed to among our fellow humans. But animals sending each other false signals isn't as straightforward as it seems. Evolution should, in theory, steer species towards honest displays and communication. If they aren't honest, natural selection will over time lead the recipient to ignore the false signals (think "the boy who cried wolf"). Game theory offers an explanation for how cheaters can succeed: if a signal is generally reliable (say, if you're a mourning cuttlefish, color pattern as an indication of sex), then a small number of cheaters can trust that they'll be trusted. Nearly 40% of males in the presence of one female and one rival tried to steal away the female. Brown, Garwood, and Williamson suggest that males shied away from attempting a cheat when there were other, larger males around that might catch them in the act. Among many animals, social punishment is generally a big deterrent for would-be fakers, and cuttlefish, it would seem, are no exception to this rule.

While this study offers an exciting snapshot at a fascinating behavior, there's still more to learn about these cuttlefishes' trickery. For example, we don't know for sure which male's sperm goes on to fertilize the female's eggs. Cuttlefish are thought to be capable of storing multiple males' sperm and later choosing which she will use (known as "cryptic mate choice"). It's possible that the female chooses the sperm from the larger, stronger male that fought for her over sperm from the smaller, weaker cheater. The study did not extensively explore other means by which cuttlefish might tell the sexes apart; it is possible that there are pheromonal cues, for instance, that the study does not control for. In addition, the study is limited in scope by its geographic confines; the behavior's context and frequency could differ in other cuttlefish populations.

This is an exciting discovery for many reasons. It gives us greater insight into how false signaling and deception play out between animals, a phenomenon not yet fully understood. It also serves as one of very few examples of deceptive tactics in invertebrates, and further illustrates to us how cognitively advanced cephalopods really are. We still have a lot to learn about cephalopods: how complex social interaction shapes their evolutionary path, what's happening at the neurological level for these animals when they solve problems (cephalopod brains are almost entirely alien to us in their structure), and to what degree females really do choose "brains over brawn" as they appear to in this study. Hopefully future research will further our understanding in these areas.

Author: Matthew

Originally written as an assignment for the University of Melbourne's open online Animal Behavior class.
0 Comments

Your comment will be posted after it is approved.


Leave a Reply.

    Welcome

    With Every Drop is a Chicago-based blog, published by CR² team members, that focuses on the biodiversity, ecology, and conservation of marine and freshwater ecosystems.  

    “Even if you never have the chance to see or touch the ocean, the ocean touches you with every breath you take, every drop of water you drink, every bite you consume. Everyone, everywhere is inextricably connected to and utterly dependent upon the existence of the sea.” – Dr. Sylvia Earle 

    Archives

    July 2015
    June 2015
    May 2015
    April 2015
    October 2014
    August 2014
    April 2014
    March 2014
    February 2014
    January 2014
    August 2013
    July 2013
    June 2013
    May 2013
    April 2013
    March 2013
    February 2013
    January 2013
    August 2012

    Categories

    All
    Adopt-a-Beach
    Cephalopod Of The Week
    Demotivational
    Digital Dispatch
    Dominican Republic Trip
    General
    Underwater Robotics
    Week 1: Squid Anatomy
    Week 2: Mission Marine Habitat
    Week 3: Corals
    Week 4: Fish Pathology
    Week 5: Lakeshore Biology
    Xkcd

    RSS Feed


    Large Visitor Globe
Powered by Create your own unique website with customizable templates.